What is Learner Autonomy and How Can it be Fostered?
by Dimitrios Thanasoulas, Greece
http://www3.telus.net/linguisticsissues/learnerautonomy.html
1.
Introduction
Over the last two decades, the concepts of learner autonomy and independence have gained momentum, the former becoming a buzz-word within the context of language learning (Little, 1991: 2). It is a truism that one of the most important spin-offs of more communicatively oriented language learning and teaching has been the premium placed on the role of the learner in the language learning process (see Wenden, 1998: xi). It goes without saying, of course, that this shift of locus of responsibility from teachers to learners does not exist in a vacuum, but is the result of a concatenation of changes to the curriculum itself towards a more learner-centred kind of learning. What is more, this reshaping, so to speak, of teacher and learner roles has been conducive to a radical change in the age-old distribution of power and authority that used to plague the traditional classroom. Cast in a new perspective and regarded as having the capacity for detachment, critical reflection, decision-making, and independent action (Little, 1991: 4), learners, autonomous learners, that is, are expected to assume greater responsibility for, and take charge of, their own learning. However, learner autonomy does not mean that the teacher becomes redundant, abdicating his / her control over what is transpiring in the language learning process.
In the present study, it will be shown that learner autonomy is a perennial dynamic process amenable to educational interventions (Candy, 1991), rather than a static product, a state, which is reached once and for all. Besides, what permeates this study is the belief that in order to help learners to assume greater control over their own learning it is important to help them to become aware of and identify the strategies that they already use or could potentially use (Holmes & Ramos, 1991, cited in James & Garrett, 1991: 198). At any rate, individual learners differ in their learning habits, interests, needs, and motivation, and develop varying degrees of independence throughout their lives (Tumposky, 1982).
2. What is
autonomy?
For a definition of autonomy, we might quote Holec (1981: 3, cited in Benson & Voller, 1997: 1) who describes it as the ability to take charge of ones learning. On a general note, the term autonomy has come to be used in at least five ways (see Benson & Voller, 1997: 2):
a) for situations in which learners study entirely on their own;
b) for a set of skills which can be learned and applied in self-directed learning;
c) for an inborn capacity which is suppressed by institutional education;
d) for the exercise of learners responsibility for their own learning;
e) for the right of learners to determine the direction of their own learning.
It is noteworthy that autonomy can be thought of in terms of a departure from education as a social process, as well as in terms of redistribution of power attending the construction of knowledge and the roles of the participants in the learning process. The relevant literature is riddled with innumerable definitions of autonomy and other synonyms for it, such as independence (Sheerin, 1991), language awareness (Lier, 1996; James & Garrett, 1991), self-direction (Candy, 1991), andragogy (Knowles, 1980; 1983a) etc., which testifies to the importance attached to it by scholars. Let us review some of these definitions and try to gain insights into what learner autonomy means and consists of.
As has been intimated so far, the term autonomy has sparked considerable controversy, inasmuch as linguists and educationalists have failed to reach a consensus as to what autonomy really is. For example, in David Littles terms, learner autonomy is essentially a matter of the learners psychological relation to the process and content of learning a capacity for detachment, critical reflection, decision-making, and independent action (Little, 1991: 4). It is not something done to learners; therefore, it is far from being another teaching method (ibid.). In the same vein, Leni Dam (1990, cited in Gathercole, 1990: 16), drawing upon Holec (1983), defines autonomy in terms of the learners willingness and capacity to control or oversee her own learning. More specifically, she, like Holec, holds that someone qualifies as an autonomous learner when he independently chooses aims and purposes and sets goals; chooses materials, methods and tasks; exercises choice and purpose in organising and carrying out the chosen tasks; and chooses criteria for evaluation.
To all intents and purposes, the autonomous learner takes a (pro-) active role in the learning process, generating ideas and availing himself of learning opportunities, rather than simply reacting to various stimuli of the teacher (Boud, 1988; Kohonen, 1992; Knowles, 1975). As we shall see, this line of reasoning operates within, and is congruent with, the theory of constructivism. For Rathbone (1971: 100, 104, cited in Candy, 1991: 271), the autonomous learner is a self-activated maker of meaning, an active agent in his own learning process. He is not one to whom things merely happen; he is the one who, by his own volition, causes things to happen. Learning is seen as the result of his own self-initiated interaction with the world.
Within such
a conception, learning is not simply a matter of rote memorisation; it is a
constructive process that involves actively seeking meaning from (or even imposing meaning
on) events (Candy, 1991: 271).
Such
inventories of characteristics evinced by the putative autonomous learner
abound, and some would say that they amount to nothing more than a romantic ideal which
does not square with reality. This stands to reason, for most of the characteristics
imputed to the autonomous learner encapsulate a wide range of attributes not
commonly associated with learners. For instance, Benn (1976, cited in Candy, 1991: 102)
likens the autonomous learner to one [w]hose life has a consistency that derives
from a coherent set of beliefs, values, and principles
[and who engages in a]
still-continuing process of criticism and re-evaluation, while Rousseau ([1762]
1911, cited in Candy, 1991: 102) regards the autonomous learner as someone who is
obedient to a law that he prescribes to himself. Within the context of education,
though, there seem to be seven main attributes characterising autonomous learners (see
Omaggio, 1978, cited in Wenden, 1998: 41-42):
1) Autonomous learners have insights into their learning styles and
strategies;
2) take an active approach to the learning task at hand;
3) are willing to take risks, i.e., to communicate in
the target language at all costs;
4) are good guessers;
5) attend to form as well as to content, that is,
place importance on accuracy as well as appropriacy;
6) develop the target language into a separate reference
system and are willing to revise and reject hypotheses and rules that do not apply;
and
7) have a tolerant and outgoing approach to the target
language.
Here, some
comments with respect to the preceding list are called for. The points briefly touched
upon above are necessary but not sufficient conditions for the development of
learner autonomy, and many more factors such as learner needs, motivation, learning
strategies, and language awareness have to be taken into consideration. For example, the
first point hinges upon a metalanguage that learners have to master in order to be
regarded as autonomous, while points 4) and 7) pertain to learner motivation. In view of
this, an attempt will be made, in subsequent sections, to shed some light on some of the
parameters affecting, and interfering with, learners self-image as well as their
capacity and will to learn. It is of consequence to note that autonomy is a process, not a
product. One does not become autonomous; one
only works towards autonomy. One corollary of
viewing autonomy in this way is the belief that there are some things to be achieved by the learner, as well as some ways of achieving these things, and that autonomy
is learned at least partly through educational experiences [and interventions]
(Candy, 1991: 115). But prior to sifting through the literature and discussing learning
strategies, motivation, and attitudes entertained by learners, it would be pertinent to
cast learner autonomy in relation to dominant philosophical approaches to learning. The
assumption is that what is dubbed as learner autonomy and the extent to which it is a
permissible and viable educational goal are all too often based on [and thus
constrained by] particular conceptions of the constitution of knowledge itself
(Benson, 1997, cited in Benson & Voller, 1997: 20).
3. Learner autonomy and dominant philosophies of
learning
In this
section, three dominant approaches to knowledge and learning will be briefly discussed,
with a view to examining how each of them connects up with learner autonomy. Positivism,
which reigned supreme in the twentieth century, is premised upon the assumption that
knowledge reflects objective reality. Therefore, if teachers can be said to hold this
objective reality, learning can only consist
in the transmission of
knowledge from one individual to another (Benson & Voller, 1997: 20). Congruent
with this view, of course, is the maintenance and enhancement of the traditional
classroom, where teachers are the purveyors of
knowledge and wielders of power, and learners are seen as container[s] to be filled
with the knowledge held by teachers (ibid.). On the other hand, positivism also
lends support to the widespread notion that knowledge is attained by dint of the
hypothesis-testing model, and that it is more effectively acquired when
it is discovered rather than taught (ibid.) (my italics). It takes little
perspicacity to realise that positivism is incongruent with, and even runs counter to, the
development of learner autonomy, as the latter refers to a gradual but radical divorce
from conventions and restrictions and is inextricably related to self-direction and
self-evaluation.
Constructivism
is an elusive concept and, within applied linguistics, is strongly associated with
Halliday (1979, cited in Benson & Voller, 1997: 21). As Candy (1991: 254) observes,
[o]ne of the central tenets of constructivism is that individuals try to give
meaning to, or construe, the perplexing maelstrom of events and ideas in which they find
themselves caught up. In contrast to positivism, constructivism posits the view
that, rather than internalising or discovering objective knowledge (whatever that might
mean), individuals reorganise and restructure their experience. In Candys terms
(Candy, 1991: 270), constructivism leads directly to the proposition that knowledge
cannot be taught but only learned (that is, constructed), because knowledge is
something built up by the learner (von Glasersfeld & Smock, 1974: xvi,
cited in Candy, 1991: 270). By the same token, language learning does not involve
internalising sets of rules, structures and forms; each learner brings her own experience
and world knowledge to bear on the target language or task at hand. Apparently,
constructivism supports, and extends to cover, psychological versions of autonomy that
appertain to learners behaviour, attitudes, motivation, and self-concept (see Benson
& Voller, 1997: 23). As a result, constructivist approaches encourage and promote
self-directed learning as a necessary condition for learner autonomy.
Finally,
critical theory, an approach within the humanities and language studies, shares with
constructivism the view that knowledge is constructed rather than discovered or learned.
Moreover, it argues that knowledge does not reflect reality, but rather comprises
competing ideological versions of that reality expressing the interests of different
social groups (Benson & Voller, 1997: 22). Within this approach, learning
concerns issues of power and ideology and is seen as a process of interaction with social
context, which can bring about social change. What is more, linguistic forms are bound up
with the social meanings they convey, in so far as language is power, and vice versa.
Certainly, learner autonomy assumes a more social and political character within critical
theory. As learners become aware of the social context in which their learning is embedded
and the constraints the latter implies, they gradually become independent, dispel myths,
disabuse themselves of preconceived ideas, and can be thought of as authors of their
own worlds (ibid.: 53).
4. Conditions for learner autonomy
The concern
of the present study has so far been with outlining the general characteristics of
autonomy. At this juncture, it should be reiterated that autonomy is not an article of
faith, a product ready made for use or merely a personal quality or trait. Rather, it
should be clarified that autonomous learning is achieved when certain conditions obtain:
cognitive and metacognitive strategies on the part of the learner, motivation, attitudes,
and knowledge about language learning, i.e., a kind of metalanguage. To acknowledge,
however, that learners have to follow certain paths
to attain autonomy is tantamount to asserting that there has to be a teacher on whom
it will be incumbent to show the way. In other words, autonomous learning is by no means
teacherless learning. As Sheerin (1997, cited in Benson & Voller, 1997:
63) succinctly puts it, [t]eachers
have a crucial role to play in launching
learners into self-access and in lending them a regular helping hand to stay afloat (my italics).
Probably,
giving students a helping hand may put paid to learner autonomy, and this is
mainly because teachers are ill-prepared or reluctant to wean [students]
away
from teacher dependence (Sheerin, 1997, cited in Benson & Voller, 1997: 63).
After all, it is not easy for teachers to change their role from purveyor of
information to counsellor and manager of learning resources
And it is not easy for
teachers to let learners solve problems for themselves (Little, 1990, cited in
Gathercole, 1990: 11). Such a transition from teacher-control to learner-control is
fraught with difficulties but it is mainly in relation to the former (no matter how
unpalatable this may sound) that the latter finds its expression. At any rate,
learner-controlwhich is ancillary to autonomyis not a single, unitary
concept, but rather a continuum along which various instructional situations may be
placed (Candy, 1991: 205). It is to these instructional situations that
we will turn in the next section. In this section, it is of utmost importance to gain
insights into the strategies learners use in grappling with the object of enquiry, i.e.,
the target language, as well as their motivation and attitude towards language learning in
general. A question germane to the discussion is, what does it mean to be an autonomous
learner in a language learning environment?
4.1.
Learning
strategies
A central
research project on learning strategies is the one surveyed in OMalley and Chamot
(1990). According to them, learning strategies are the special thoughts or behaviors
that individuals use to help them comprehend, learn, or retain new information
(OMalley and Chamot, 1990: 1, cited in Cook, 1993: 113)a definition in keeping
with the one provided in Wenden (1998: 18): Learning
strategies are mental steps or operations that learners use to learn a new language
and to regulate their efforts to do so. To a greater or lesser degree, the
strategies and learning styles that someone adopts may partly reflect personal
preference rather than innate endowment (Skehan, 1998: 237). We will only briefly
discuss some of the main learning strategies, refraining from mentioning communication or
compensatory strategies (see Cook, 1993 for more details).
4.1.1.
Cognitive
strategies
According to
OMalley and Chamot (1990: 44), cognitive strategies operate directly on
incoming information, manipulating it in ways that enhance learning. Learners may
use any or all of the following cognitive strategies (see Cook, 1993: 114-115):
a) repetition, when imitating others speech;
b) resourcing, i.e., having recourse to dictionaries
and other materials;
c) translation, that is, using their mother tongue as
a basis for understanding and / or producing the target language;
d) note-taking;
e) deduction, i.e., conscious application of L2
rules;
f) contextualisation, when embedding a word or phrase
in a meaningful sequence;
g) transfer, that is, using knowledge acquired in the
L1 to remember and understand facts and sequences in the L2;
h) inferencing, when matching an unfamiliar word
against available information (a new word etc);
i) question
for clarification, when asking the teacher to explain, etc.
There are
many more cognitive strategies in the relevant literature. OMalley and Chamot (1990)
recognise 16.
4.1.2.
Metacognitive
strategies
According to
Wenden (1998: 34), metacognitive knowledge includes all facts learners acquire about
their own cognitive processes as they are applied and used to gain knowledge and acquire
skills in varied situations. In a sense, metacognitive strategies are skills used
for planning, monitoring, and evaluating the learning activity; they are strategies about learning rather than learning strategies
themselves (Cook, 1993: 114). Let us see some of these strategies:
a) directed attention, when deciding in advance to
concentrate on general aspects of a task;
b) selective attention, paying attention to specific
aspects of a task;
c) self-monitoring, i.e., checking ones
performance as one speaks;
d) self-evaluation, i.e., appraising ones
performance in relation to ones own standards;
e) self-reinforcement, rewarding oneself for success.
At the planning stage, also known as pre-planning (see Wenden, 1998: 27), learners
identify their objectives and determine how they will achieve them. Planning, however, may
also go on while a task is being performed. This is called planning-in-action. Here, learners may change
their objectives and reconsider the ways in which they will go about achieving them. At
the monitoring stage, language learners act as
participant observers or overseers of their language learning (ibid.), asking
themselves, How am I doing? Am I having difficulties with this task?, and so
on. Finally, when learners evaluate, they do so
in terms of the outcome of their attempt to use
a certain strategy. According to Wenden (1998: 28), evaluating involves three steps: 1)
learners examine the outcome of their attempts to learn; 2) they access the criteria they
will use to judge it; and 3) they apply it.
4.2.
Learner
attitudes and motivation
Language
learning is not merely a cognitive task. Learners do not only reflect on their learning in
terms of the language input to which they are exposed, or the optimal strategies they need
in order to achieve the goals they set. Rather, the success of a learning activity is, to
some extent, contingent upon learners stance towards the world and the learning
activity in particular, their sense of self, and their desire to learn (see Benson &
Voller, 1997: 134-136). As Candy (1991: 295-296) says, the how and the what
of learning are intimately interwoven
[T]he overall approach a learner adopts
will significantly influence the shape of his or her learning outcomes (my italics).
In other words, language learningas well as learning, in generalhas also an affective component. Meeting and
interiorising the grammar of a foreign language is not simply an intelligent, cognitive
act. It is a highly affective one too
(Rinvolucri, 1984: 5, cited in James
& Garrett, 1991: 13). Gardner and MacIntyre (1993: 1, cited in Graham, 1997: 92)
define affective variables as the emotionally relevant characteristics
of the individual that influence how she / he will respond to any situation. Other
scholars, such as Shumann (1978) and Larsen-Freeman and Long (1991) attach less importance
to learners emotions, claiming that social and psychological factors
give a more suitable description for students reactions to the learning process.
Amongst the social and affective variables at work, self-esteem and desire to learn are
deemed to be the most crucial factors in the learners ability to overcome
occasional setbacks or minor mistakes in the process of learning a second [or foreign]
language (Tarone & Yule, 1989: 139). In this light, it is necessary to shed some
light on learner attitudes and motivation.
Wenden
(1998: 52) defines attitudes as learned motivations, valued beliefs, evaluations,
what one believes is acceptable, or responses oriented towards approaching or
avoiding. For her, two kinds of attitudes are crucial: attitudes learners hold about
their role in the learning process, and their capability as learners (ibid.: 53). In a sense,
attitudes are a form of metacognitive knowledge. At any rate, learner beliefs about
their role and capability as learners will be shaped and maintained
by other beliefs
they hold about themselves as learners (ibid.: 54). For example, if learners believe
that certain personality types cannot learn a foreign language and they believe that they
are that type of person, then they will think that they are fighting a losing
battle, as far as learning the foreign language is concerned. Furthermore, if
learners labour under the misconception that learning is successful only within the
context of the traditional classroom, where the teacher directs, instructs,
and manages the learning activity, and students must follow in the teachers
footsteps, they are likely to be impervious or resistant to learner-centred strategies
aiming at autonomy, and success is likely to be undermined.
In a way,
attitudes are part of ones perception of self, of others, and of the culture
in which one is living [or the culture of the target language] (Brown, 1987: 126),
and it seems clear that positive attitudes are conducive to increased motivation, while
negative attitudes have the opposite effect. But let us examine the role of motivation.
Although the
term motivation is frequently used in educational contexts, there is little
agreement among experts as to its exact meaning. What most scholars seem to agree on,
though, is that motivation is one of the key factors that influence the rate and
success of second / foreign language (L2) learning. Motivation provides the primary
impetus to initiate learning the L2 and later the driving force to sustain the long and
often tedious learning process (Dornyei, 1998: 117). According to Gardner and
MacIntyre (1993: 3), motivation is comprised of three components: desire to achieve
a goal, effort extended in this direction, and satisfaction with the task.
It is manifest
that in language learning, people are motivated in different ways and to different
degrees. Some learners like doing grammar and memorising; others want to speak and
role-play; others prefer reading and writing, while avoiding speaking. Furthermore, since
[the learning of a foreign language] involves an alteration in self-image, the
adoption of new social and cultural behaviours and ways of being, and therefore has a
significant impact on the social nature of the learner (Williams, 1994: 77, cited in
Dornyei, 1998: 122), an important distinction should be made between instrumental and integrative motivation. Learners with an
instrumental orientation view the foreign language as a means of finding a good job or
pursuing a lucrative career; in other words, the target language acts as a monetary
incentive (Gardner & MacIntyre, 1993: 3). On the other hand, learners with an
integrative orientation are interested in the culture of the target language; they want to
acquaint themselves with the target community and become integral parts of it. Of course,
this approach to motivation has certain limitations (see Cookes and Schmidt, 1991, cited
in Lier, 1996: 104-105), but an in-depth analysis is not within the purview of this study.
The bottom line is that motivation is a central mediator in the prediction of
language achievement (Gardner & MacIntyre, 1993: 3), as various studies have
shown (see Kraemer, 1990; Machnick and Wolfe, 1982; et
al.).
4.3.
Self-esteem
Closely
related to attitudes and motivation is the concept of self-esteem, that is, the evaluation
the learner makes of herself with regard to the target language or learning in general.
[S]elf-esteem is a personal judgement of worthiness that is expressed in the
attitudes that the individual holds towards himself (Coopersmith, 1967: 4-5, cited
in Brown, 1987: 101-102). If the learner has a robust sense of self, to quote
Breen and Mann (1997, cited in Benson & Voller, 1997: 134), his relationship to
himself as a learner is unlikely to be marred by any negative assessments by the teacher.
Conversely, a lack of self-esteem is likely to lead to negative attitudes towards his
capability as a learner, and to a deterioration in cognitive performance, thus
confirming his view of himself as incapable of learning (Diener and Dweck, 1978, 1980,
cited in Wenden, 1998: 57).
Now that we
have examined some of the factors that may enhance, or even militate against, the
learners willingness to take charge of her own learning and her confidence in her
ability as a learner, it is of consequence to consider possible ways of promoting learner
autonomy. To say, though, that learner autonomy can be fostered is not to reduce it to a
set of skills that need to be acquired. Rather, it is taken to mean that the teacher and
the learner can work towards autonomy by creating a friendly atmosphere characterised by
low threat, unconditional positive regard, honest and open feedback, respect for the
ideas and opinions of others, approval of self-improvement as a goal, collaboration rather
than competition (Candy, 1991: 337). In the next section, some general guidelines
for promoting learner autonomy will be given, on the assumption that the latter does not
mean leaving learners to their own devices or learning in isolation.
5. How can learner autonomy be promoted?
To posit
ways of fostering learner autonomy is certainly to posit ways of fostering teacher autonomy, as [t]eachers
autonomy permeates into [learners] autonomy (Johnson, Pardesi and Paine, 1990,
cited in Gathercole, 1990: 51). Nevertheless, our main focus will be on what the learner
can do in order to attain a considerable degree of autonomy, even though the success of
the learner is, to a great extent, determinedalas! vitiatedby the educational
system and the requisite role of the teacher.
5.1.
Self-reports
According to
Wenden (1998: 79-95), a good way of collecting information on how students go about a
learning task and helping them become aware of their own strategies is to assign a task
and have them report what they are thinking while they
are performing it. This self-report is called introspective,
as learners are asked to introspect on their learning. In this case, the
[introspective] self-report is a verbalization of ones stream of consciousness
(Wenden, 1998: 81). Introspective reports are assumed to provide information on the
strategies learners are using at the time of the report. However, this method suffers from
one limitation: [t]he concentration put on thinking aloud might detract from
[learners] ability to do the task efficiently (ibid.: 83), thus rendering the
outcome of the report spurious and tentative.
Another type of
self-report is what has been dubbed as retrospective
self-report, since learners are asked to think back or retrospect on their learning.
Retrospective self-reports are quite open ended, in that there is no limit put on what
students say in response to a question or statement that points to a topic in a general
way. There are two kinds of retrospective self-reports: semi-structured interviews and structured questionnaires. A semi-structured
interview may focus on a specific skill with a view to extracting information about
learners feelings towards particular skills (reading, listening, etc.), problems
encountered, techniques resorted to in order to tackle these problems, and learners
views on optimal strategies or ways of acquiring specific skills or dealing with learning
tasks. A structured questionnaire seeks the same information but in a different way: by
dint of explicit questions and statements, and then asking learners to agree or disagree,
write true or false, and so forth.
It could be
argued that self-reports can be a means of raising awareness of learners strategies
and the need for constant evaluation of techniques, goals, and outcomes. As Wenden (1998:
90) observes, without awareness [learners] will remain trapped in their old patterns
of beliefs and behaviors and never be fully autonomous.
5.2.
Diaries
and evaluation sheets
Perhaps one
of the principal goals of education is to alter learners beliefs about themselves by
showing them that their putative failures or shortcomings can be ascribed to a lack of
effective strategies rather than to a lack of potential. After all, according to Vygotsky
(1978), learning is an internalised form of a formerly social activity, and a
learner can realize [his] potential interactivelythrough the guidance of supportive
other persons such as parents, teachers, and peers (Wenden, 1998: 107). Herein lies
the role of diaries and evaluation sheets, which offer students the possibility to plan,
monitor, and evaluate their learning, identifying any problems they run into and
suggesting solutions. Let us have a look at the following diaries based on authentic
student accounts of their language learning:
A.
Dear Diary,
These first few days have been terrible. I studied English for eight
years
just think, eight years, but I only learned a lot of grammar. I cant
speak a word. I dont dare. I cant express myself in the right way, so I am
afraid to speak.
The other day I started watching TV, so I could get accustomed to the sound. I
dont understand TV news very well
only a few words. I cant get the main
point. In school its easy to understand, but I cant understand the people in
the stores.
What can I do?
Yours Truly,
Impatient
(from Wenden, 1998: 102)
B.
Dear Diary,
I read the New York Times every day.
Every day I learn many new expressionsa lot of vocabulary. But I cant use this
vocabulary in conversation. The same thing happens with what I learn at school. I
cant use it when I want to talk to Americans or even with my own Spanish friends.
I need some help.
Yours Truly,
Confused
(from Wenden, 1998: 102)
Alongside
diaries, students can also benefit from putting pen to paper and writing on their
expectations of a course at the beginning of term, and then filling in evaluation sheets,
or reporting on the outcomes of a course, at the end of term. These activities are bound
to help learners put things into perspective and manage their learning more effectively.
Let us consider two such reports:
1.
What do I want to do this year?
I want to speak more English and Id like to
spell better that I do now. I would like to work with another boy or girl who is willing
to speak English with me and make some activities in English. Materials: Challenge to
think and crosswords.
I
would like to get a more varied language and I would like to be better at spelling,
especially the words used in everyday situations. How: I will prepare two
minutes talk for every lesson, I will write down new words five times and
practise pronouncing them. I will get someone or myself to correct it. I will read at
least two booksdifficult onesand make book-reviews.
(Beginning of term4th year of English
[from Dam, 1990, cited in Gathercole, 1990: 30])
2.
What do you feel you know now that you didnt
know before?
I think that we have grown better at
planning our own time. We know more about what we need to do and how to go about it. We
try all the time to extend our vocabulary and to get an active language. Evaluation also
helped us. It is like going through things again.
(End of term4th year of English
[from Dam, 1990, cited in Gathercole, 1990: 32])
So far, one of the assumptions underlying this discussion on learner autonomy has been that the teacher has not relinquished his authority; rather, that he has committed himself to providing the learners with the opportunity to experiment, make hypotheses, and improvise, in their attempt to master the target language and, along with it, to learn how to learn in their own, individual, holistic way (see Papaconstantinou, 1997). It may be the case that learner autonomy is best achieved when, among other things, the teacher acts as a facilitator of learning, a counsellor, and as a resource (see Voller, 1997, cited in Benson and Voller, 1997: 99-106). In other words, when she lies somewhere along a continuum between what Barnes (1976, cited in Benson and Voller, 1997: 99) calls transmission and interpretation teachers. As Wright (1987: 62, cited in Benson and Voller, 1997: 100) notes, transmission teachers believe in subject disciplines and boundaries between them, in content, in standards of performance laid down by these disciplines that can be objectively evaluated that learners will find it hard to meet the standards; interpretation teachers believe that knowledge is the ability to organize thought, interpret and act on facts; that learners are intrinsically interested and naturally inclined to explore their worlds that learners already know a great deal and have the ability to refashion that knowledge.
The
interpretation teacher respects learners needs and is more likely to follow a fraternal-permissive model (emphasis added)
(Stevick, 1976: 91-93, cited in Benson and Voller, 1997: 100). It is with this type of
teacher that the role of persuasive communication is most congruent.
5.3.
Persuasive
communication as a means of altering learner beliefs and attitudes
Inasmuch as
the success of learning and the extent to which learners tap into their potential
resources in order to overcome difficulties and achieve autonomy are determined by such
factors as learners motivation, their desire to learn, and the beliefs they hold
about themselves as learners and learning per se,
it is manifest that changing some negative beliefs and attitudes is bound to
facilitate learning. Attitude change [is assumed to] be brought about through
exposure to a persuasive communication [between the teacher and the learners]
(Wenden, 1998: 126). According to
the Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM) of attitude change developed by Petty and Cacciopo
(1986, cited in Wenden, 1998: 126), there are several ways of bringing about this change,
however, our concern will only be with persuasive communication.
A persuasive
communication is a discussion presenting information and arguments to change a
learners evaluation of a topic, situation, task, and so on. These arguments could be
either explicit or implicit, especially when the topic is deemed of importance. If, for
instance, a deeply ingrained fear or belief precludes the learner from engaging in the
learning process, persuasive communication purports to help bring these facts to light and
identify the causes that underlie them. It should be noted, though, that no arguments to
influence students views are given. Rather, the communication comprises facts that
show what learners can do to attain autonomy and that learners who do so are successful
(see Wenden, 1998: 126). This approach is based on the assumption that when learners are
faced with convincing information about a situation, they can be led to re-examine
existing evaluations they hold about it and revise or change them completely (ibid.:
127).
6. Conclusion
This study is far from comprehensive, as we have only skimmed the surface of the subject and the puzzle called learner autonomy. Many more pieces are missing. For instance, no mention has been made of the role of the curriculum in promoting learner autonomy, despite the debate on the relationship between classroom practice and ideological encoding (Littlejohn, 1997, cited in Benson and Voller, 1997: 181-182). At any rate, the main point of departure for this study has been the notion that there are degrees of learner autonomy and that it is not an absolute concept. It would be nothing short of ludicrous to assert that learners come into the learning situation with the knowledge and skills to plan, monitor, and evaluate their learning, or to make decisions on content or objectives. Nevertheless, learner autonomy is an ideal, so to speak, that can, and should, be realised, if we want self-sufficient learners and citizens capable of evaluating every single situation they find themselves in and drawing the line at any inconsistencies or shortcomings in institutions and society at large. Certainly, though, autonomous learning is not akin to unbridled learning. There has to be a teacher who will adapt resources, materials, and methods to the learners needs and even abandon all this if need be. Learner autonomy consists in becoming aware of, and identifying, ones strategies, needs, and goals as a learner, and having the opportunity to reconsider and refashion approaches and procedures for optimal learning. But even if learner autonomy is amenable to educational interventions, it should be recognised that it takes a long time to develop, and simply removing the barriers to a persons ability to think and behave in certain ways may not allow him or her to break away from old habits or old ways of thinking (Candy, 1991: 124). As Holyoake (1892, vol. 1, p. 4) succinctly put it, [k]nowledge lies everywhere to hand for those who observe and think.
REFERENCES
Barnes, D. 1976. From Communication to Curriculum. Harmondsworth:Penguin.
Benn, S. I.
1976. Freedom, Autonomy and the Concept of the Person. In
ristotelian Society Proceedings, new series, pp.
109-130.
Benson, P.
& Voller, P. 1997. Autonomy and Independence in
Language
Learning. London: Longman.
Boud, D.
(ed.). 1988. Developing Student Autonomy in
Learning. New York:
Kogan Press.
Brown, H. D. 1987. Principles of Language Learning and Teaching. Englewood
Cliffs, JC: Prentice Hall.
Candy, 1991. Self-direction for Lifelong Learning. California:
Jossey-Bass.
Cook, V. 1993.
Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition. London:
Macmillan.
Coopersmith, S. 1967. The Antecedents of Self-Esteem. San Francisco: W.
H. Freeman & Company.
Crookes, G.
& Schmidt, R. 1991. Motivation: Reopening the research
agenda. Language Learning. 41: 469-512.
Dam, L. 1990.
Learner Autonomy in Practice. In Gathercole, I. (ed.). 1990,
p16. CILT. Great Britain: Bourne Press.
Dornyei, Z.
1998. Motivation in Second and Foreign Language
Learning.
CILT: CUP.
Gardner, R. C.
and MacIntyre, P. D. 1993. A Students Contributions to
Second-language Learning. Part II: Affective
variables. Language
Teaching 26, 1-11.
Graham, S. 1997. Effective Language Learning. Great Britain: WBC.
Halliday,
M.A.K. 1979. Language as Social Semiotic. London:
Edward
Arnold.
Holec, H. 1981. Autonomy in Foreign Language Learning. Oxford:
OUP.
Holmes, J. L.
and Ramos, R. 1991. Talking about learning: establishing a
framework for discussing and changing learning
processes. In James, C.
and Garrett, P. (eds.). Language
Awareness in the Classroom. 1991: 198-212).
Holyoake, J.
1892. Sixty Years of An Agitators Life. (2
vols.). London:
Unwin.
Johnson,
Pardesi, and Paine. 1990. Autonomy in Our Primary School. In
Gathercole, I. 1990. Autonomy in Language Learning. CILT: Bourne Press.
Knowles, M. S. 1975. Self-directed Learning. New York: Association
Press.
Knowles, M. S.
1980. The Modern Practice of Adult Education: From
Pedagogy to Andragogy. Chicago: Follett.
Knowles, M. S.
1983a. Andragogy: An Emerging Technology for Adult
Learning. In M. Tight (ed.), Adult Learning and Education. London:
Croom Helm.
Kohonen, V.
1992. Experiential language learning: second language learning
as cooperative learner
education. In Nunan, D. (Ed.), Collaborative
Language Learning and Teaching, pp. 14-39.
Larsen-Freeman,
D. and Long, M. H. 1991. An Introduction to Second
Language Acquisition Research. London: Longman.
Lier, Van L. 1996. Interaction
in the Language Curriculum. Awareness,
Autonomy and Authenticity. USA:
Longman.
Little, D.
1991. Learner Autonomy. 1: Definitions, Issues and
Problems.
Dublin: Authentik.
Littlejohn, A.
1997. Self-access work and curriculum ideologies. In Benson, P.
and Voller, P. (eds.). Autonomy and Independence in Language Learning.
London: Longman.
Muchnick, A.
G. and Wolfe, D. E. 1982. Attitudes and motivations of
American students of Spanish. The Canadian Modern Language Review,
38, 262-281.
Omaggio, A.
1978. Successful language learners: What do we know about
them?, ERIC / CLL News Bulletin, May, 2-3.
OMalley,
J. M. and Chamot, A. V. 1990. Learning Strategies
in Second
Language Acquisition. London: Macmillan.
Papaconstantinou,
A. 1997. Creating the Whole Person in New Age. Athens:
Kardamitsa.
Petty, R. E.
and Cacioppo, J. T. 1986. Communication and
Persuasion:
Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. New
York: Springer-Verlag.
Rathbone, C.
H. 1971. Open Education: The Informal Classroom. New
York:
Citation Press.
Rinvolucri, M.
1984. Grammar Games: Cognitive, Affective, and
Drama
Activation for EFL Students. Cambridge: CUP.
Rousseau, J. J. [1762],
1911. Emile. London: Dent.
Schumann, J.
H. 1978. Social and Psychological Factors in Second Language
Acquisition. In J. C.
Richards (ed.). Understanding Second and Foreign
Language Learning. pp. 163-178. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Sheerin, S. 1991. State of the
art: self-access, Language Teaching, 24:
3, pp 153-157.
Sheerin, S. 1997. An Exploration of
the Relationship between Self-access and Independent Learning. In Benson, P. and Voller,
P. (eds.). 1997. Autonomy and Independence in
Language Learning. London: Longman.
Skehan, P. 1998. A Cognitive Approach to Language Learning. Oxford:
OUP.
Tarone, E. and Yule, G. 1989. Focus on the Language Learner. Oxford: OUP.
Tumposky, N.
1982. The learner on his own. In M. Geddes and G.
Sturtridge (eds.). Individualisation. London: Modern English
Publications, pp. 4-7.
Vygotsky, L. S. 1978. Mind in Society. USA: Harvard.
Wenden, A.
1998. Learner Strategies for Learner Autonomy. Great
Britain:
Prentice Hall.
Williams, M. 1994.
Motivation in Foreign and Second Language Learning:
An Interactive Perspective. Educational and Child Psychology, 11, 17-84.